Calendar An icon of a desk calendar. Cancel An icon of a circle with a diagonal line across. Caret An icon of a block arrow pointing to the right. Email An icon of a paper envelope. Facebook An icon of the Facebook "f" mark. Google An icon of the Google "G" mark. Linked In An icon of the Linked In "in" mark. Logout An icon representing logout. Profile An icon that resembles human head and shoulders. Telephone An icon of a traditional telephone receiver. Tick An icon of a tick mark. Is Public An icon of a human eye and eyelashes. Is Not Public An icon of a human eye and eyelashes with a diagonal line through it. Pause Icon A two-lined pause icon for stopping interactions. Quote Mark A opening quote mark. Quote Mark A closing quote mark. Arrow An icon of an arrow. Folder An icon of a paper folder. Breaking An icon of an exclamation mark on a circular background. Camera An icon of a digital camera. Caret An icon of a caret arrow. Clock An icon of a clock face. Close An icon of the an X shape. Close Icon An icon used to represent where to interact to collapse or dismiss a component Comment An icon of a speech bubble. Comments An icon of a speech bubble, denoting user comments. Comments An icon of a speech bubble, denoting user comments. Ellipsis An icon of 3 horizontal dots. Envelope An icon of a paper envelope. Facebook An icon of a facebook f logo. Camera An icon of a digital camera. Home An icon of a house. Instagram An icon of the Instagram logo. LinkedIn An icon of the LinkedIn logo. Magnifying Glass An icon of a magnifying glass. Search Icon A magnifying glass icon that is used to represent the function of searching. Menu An icon of 3 horizontal lines. Hamburger Menu Icon An icon used to represent a collapsed menu. Next An icon of an arrow pointing to the right. Notice An explanation mark centred inside a circle. Previous An icon of an arrow pointing to the left. Rating An icon of a star. Tag An icon of a tag. Twitter An icon of the Twitter logo. Video Camera An icon of a video camera shape. Speech Bubble Icon A icon displaying a speech bubble WhatsApp An icon of the WhatsApp logo. Information An icon of an information logo. Plus A mathematical 'plus' symbol. Duration An icon indicating Time. Success Tick An icon of a green tick. Success Tick Timeout An icon of a greyed out success tick. Loading Spinner An icon of a loading spinner. Facebook Messenger An icon of the facebook messenger app logo. Facebook An icon of a facebook f logo. Facebook Messenger An icon of the Twitter app logo. LinkedIn An icon of the LinkedIn logo. WhatsApp Messenger An icon of the Whatsapp messenger app logo. Email An icon of an mail envelope. Copy link A decentered black square over a white square.

Dundonian dialect at centre of international language study

Dr Neil Kirk
Dr Neil Kirk

The distinctive Dundonian brogue is at the centre of new international research into how our minds process dialects and languages.

Academics from Abertay University partnered with RWTH Aachen University, Germany, to study how quickly the brain reacts when asked to switch between standard speech and regional dialects.

During the study, participants were given a list of both English and Dundonian words which then appeared on a colour-coded screen in randomised order.

Depending on the colour, they were asked to say the word in either English or Dundonian – for example, responding “house” if the image was coloured green or “hoose” if the image was blue.

Other words included in the survey were girl/lassie, armpit/oxter, heart/hert, sausages/sassages, ears/lugs, and children/bairns.

Abertay University

Researchers at RWTH, researchers carried out the same experiment with people using standard German and the regional Öcher Platt dialect.

The results suggest different dialects (or closely related language varieties) are stored in the brain in similar ways to different languages.

Project leader Dr Neil Kirk, of Abertay’s Division of Psychology, said: “In most other studies our bidialectal participants would simply be considered ‘monolingual’, as language background questionnaires typically do not enquire about dialect usage.

“Yet the results of our study show that some monolinguals and bilinguals are cognitively not that different.”

Both sets of researchers found a “switch cost”, where it took participants longer to name pictures when they were asked to move from speaking one variety to another.

They also discovered that this cost remained the same for people comfortable with both English and Dundonian, regardless of which direction the switch went in.

However, for those with one language stronger than the other – in this case English participants with little or no previous experience of Dundonian – the cost was greater when reverting back to speaking English.

Dr Kirk, who conducted the research as part of his PhD, added: “One explanation for this is that both varieties are always active, but in order to speak one of them, you need to suppress or inhibit the other variety.

“More cognitive effort is required to suppress a stronger variety and this creates a delay in being able to activate it again.”